Skip to main content

Public perception of the lifetime morbid risk of mental disorders in the United States and associations with public stigma

Abstract

Purpose

This study examined the relationship between estimates of the prevalence of mental disorders and mental health stigma. It also examined whether stigma might be more greatly associated with the terms “mental illness,” “mental disorder,” or “mental health condition.”

Methods

Respondents (N = 302) on Amazon’s Mechanical Turk completed an online survey designed to measure social distance, which is one variant of stigma. Half of the respondents were informed at the beginning of the survey that the lifetime morbid risk (LMR) of meeting criteria for at least one mental disorder at some point in life was 70–80 %, while the others were asked to provide their own LMR estimates. All respondents were also randomly assigned to view the survey with either the term “mental illness,” “mental disorder,” or “mental health condition.”

Results

Higher LMR estimates (B = −0.030; β = −0.154), having a mental disorder (B = −2.002; β = −0.200), and a history of contact with an individual with a mental disorder (B = −2.812; β = −0.298), each significantly predicted lower desire for social distance. Respondents in the “mental disorder” group endorsed greater desire for social distance. Participants who were informed about LMR at the start of the survey did not score lower on social distance.

Conclusions

Estimates for LMR were more than half as predictive of social distance scores as contact with individuals with mental disorders. But anti-stigma interventions may need to do more than inform individuals about the high prevalence of mental disorders in order to be effective.

Background

Mental health stigma

The majority of Americans will meet criteria for at least one mental disorder by age 29 (Kessler and Wang 2008). Yet the stigma associated with these conditions is considerable, and may contribute even greater to the morbidity of a condition than the disorder itself (Hinshaw and Stier 2008). Global agencies, such as the World Health Organization and World Psychiatric Organization, national programs, and other leading authorities have identified stigma as the biggest problem facing psychiatry today (Sartorius 2004; World Health Organization 2001; World Psychiatric Association 2016; Young Minds 2010).

In surveys, US employers express more negative attitudes about hiring workers with psychiatric disabilities than almost any other group (Cook 2006), and 43.8 % are uncomfortable hiring those in treatment for depression (Scheid 1999). Worldwide, the public stigma of mental disorders is widely prevalent, even among mental health professionals (Nordt et al. 2006; Schulze 2007), and components of stigma have now been linked empirically with treatment avoidance (Clement et al. 2015; Mojtabai 2010).

The stigma of mental disorders is somewhat difficult to understand in the face of evidence that suggests their very high prevalence in the general population. By age 29, lifetime prevalence for having any mental disorder is 58.7 % by retrospective studies (National Comorbidity Survey Replication 2007), but prospective studies have found prevalence estimates to be double those of retrospective studies for individual disorders (Moffitt et al. 2010; Takayanagi et al. 2014). Lifetime morbid risk (LMR), or the probability of meeting criteria for a mental disorder at any point in one’s life from birth to death, should be even higher, but there are currently no known estimates or relevant data.

Stigma and prevalence estimates

Stigma, as conceived by many theorists, provides a majority group with system-justification for discriminatory behavior against a minority (Corrigan et al. 2003; Jones and Corrigan 2014; Link and Phelan 2001; Nosek et al. 2009). Crucial to this end is the formation of stereotypes about the minority to establish its difference as the “other” group (Corrigan et al. 2003). If all, or nearly all, Americans are believed to be likely to have a “mental disorder” at some point in their lives, it should, presumably, be more difficult to sustain such perceived differences. Conceivably, the persistence of stigma might be related to a lack of public knowledge about the very high prevalence of mental disorders.

Only one previous study, Von dem Knesebeck et al. (2013), appears to have examined such a relationship. The study measured social distance, which is one particular variant of stigma. Respondents were contacted over the phone and provided vignettes describing individuals with the specific disorders of either depression, schizophrenia, anorexia nervosa, or bulimia nervosa, followed by the diagnosis. The participants were asked to estimate the LMR of the condition and to complete a survey designed to assess their desire for social distance from individuals such as those described in the vignette. The authors found that higher estimates for schizophrenia were associated with less desire for social distance, but there were no significant associations for the other disorders. A 1 % increase in estimated LMR for schizophrenia was associated with a 0.04 point decrease in scores for desired social distance (Von dem Knesebeck et al. 2013).

Yet estimates of the LMRs for individual disorders may not be related to LMR estimates for having any disorder. Some studies, including Von dem Knesebeck et al. have found that the public tends to overestimate the prevalence for specific disorders (Economou et al. 2009; Mond et al. 2004), but underestimate the prevalence for having at least one mental disorder of any type (Bourget et al. 2007; Department of Health 2003; Kemali et al. 1989). Von dem Knesebeck et al. (2013) did not find a relationship between social distance and LMR estimates for specific disorders, but the association between social distance and LMR estimates for all mental disorders has never been studied.

Terminology

At the time this study was designed, it was not clear which particular term should be used in the surveys that would be completed by participants. Terms such as “mental illness,” “mental disorder,” and “mental health condition” are used interchangeably in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fifth edition (American Psychiatric Association 2013), and Szeto et al. (2013) found that such terms were associated with similar levels of stigma. But there are still many reasons to suspect that the public might view the terms differently. For example, “mental illness” suggests severity (Szeto et al. 2013), while “mental disorder” implies a difference from normal. If social distance depends primarily on conceptualizations of the stigmatized as different (Corrigan et al. 2015), then perhaps the term “mental disorder” would be most likely to evoke the respondents’ desire for social distance. In spite of the findings from previous research, it was hypothesized that these 3 terms would significantly differ on associated social distance when measured here in this study. The term “mental disorder” will be used for the remainder of this article, unless otherwise noted.

The main hypothesis of this study was based on the presumption that respondents believing that a higher percentage of the population will meet criteria for a mental disorder at some point in life should also hold less stigmatizing views about the disorders than those who believe they are less common. Specifically, it was expected that high LMR estimates would be negatively associated with scores for social distance. It was also expected that providing information about the very high prevalence of mental disorders at the start of the survey would lead respondents to endorse less stigmatizing views of those with mental disorders.

Methods

Sample

The study was conducted through an anonymous online survey, given reports that online self-complete surveys, in contrast with face-to-face assessments, are relatively free of social desirability bias when measuring behavioral intentions toward people with mental disorders (Henderson et al. 2012). Respondents were recruited and surveys administered through Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk), which has been found to yield data at least as reliable and valid as that obtained through methods traditionally employed in psychology and the social sciences (Buhrmester et al. 2011; Paolacci and Chandler 2014; Shapiro et al. 2013). MTurk samples include only those who are 18 years or older, have demographics that generally resemble the US, though with some differences, and model a random population (Paolacci and Chandler 2014; Paolacci et al. 2010; Shapiro et al. 2013).

Three hundred participants were recruited. A notice was posted on the MTurk Human Intelligence Tasks list requesting workers to “answer a survey about mental health” that would take no more than 15 min to complete. All tasks were completed within hours of the posted notice. Workers on MTurk were only eligible to participate if they resided in the US, had completed at least 50 prior assignments on MTurk, and had a prior task approval rating of at least 95 %. These requirements are standard for research with MTurk populations and ensured high quality data by excluding individuals who might end the survey midway and thereby bias the results. Participants who successfully completed the survey were paid $0.40. In general, however, MTurk workers are internally motivated and will readily participate in research of this kind for much less than this amount (Buhrmester et al. 2011). The study was determined to be exempt from review by an Institutional Review Board.

Demographics

Respondents answered a number of items on demographics. For contact, they were asked if they had ever known someone with a mental disorder. For personal experience, respondents were asked, “Do you have a mental disorder?” and affirmative responses were considered to indicate personal experience with a mental disorder. The answers were based on the perception of the informant, the same approach taken by Von dem Knesebeck et al. (2013). In other studies, however, only those told they had a mental health diagnosis by a mental health provider were considered to have a disorder, and some studies also required a history of treatment. Yet 67 % of those with mental disorders do not receive treatment (Kessler et al. 2005), and the approach taken here therefore seemed more likely to capture those with the variable of interest.

Table 1 summarizes the characteristics of the overall sample, which were consistent with the typical demographic profiles of MTurk. Participants ranged in age from 18 to 75. Compared with the general US population, the respondents in this study were younger, with a higher proportion of males and whites, more highly educated, and also more likely to be unemployed. The mean of respondents’ estimates for the LMR of having any mental disorder was 32.95 (SD = 19.89).

Table 1 Demographics of all respondents (N = 302)

Measures

To determine if a particular term was more associated with desire for social distance, participants were randomly assigned to view the survey with either the term “mental illness,” “mental disorder,” or “mental health condition.” Half the participants (n = 151) were informed at the beginning of the survey that the LMR of having at least one mental disorder for an individual in the US is 70–80 %, in order to see if this had any effect on stigma scores. The other half (n = 151) were not provided with this information, and they were asked, instead, to provide their own estimates. They were asked, “What percentage of the current US population has, has had, or will have a mental disorder in their lifetime?” This resulted in 6 distinct groups, as seen in Table 2.

Table 2 Mean (and standard deviation) social distance scores by terminology group among those informed and those not informed of prevalence estimates

The instrument used to measure social distance was adopted from the Stigma in Global Context—Mental Health Study (SGC–MHS) and the General Social Survey (GSS) (Bogardus 1959; Martin et al. 2000; Pescosolido et al. 2013; Phillips 1963; Smith et al. 2015). The SGC–MHS/GSS instrument has been used on tens of thousands of respondents, and was developed through an international collaboration involving 16 countries. The reliability (Cronbach’s α) for a 6-item social distance scale from the GSS was found in one study to be .87 (Martin et al. 2000); the items, as used in this study, had a Cronbach’s α of .91. Social distance, the most commonly measured aspect of stigma, refers to the reluctance to interact with members of devalued groups. Social distance items ask respondents to indicate their willingness to have someone with a mental disorder as a neighbor, a friend, a co-worker, an in-law, and so on. All items were coded in this study such that more stigmatizing attitudes receive higher scores, and were administered on a 4-point Likert scale. The items used here and their wording are available as Additional file 1.

Design and analyses

Multiple linear regression analyses were performed on SPSS 19 to examine the relationship between prevalence estimates and social distance. The analyses were performed only on the respondents asked to provide LMR estimates (n = 151), who were not informed about LMR at the start of the survey. The candidate predictors selected for the regression were chosen to facilitate comparisons with Von dem Knesebeck et al. Six candidate predictors (gender, age, educational attainment, personal experience with a mental disorder, contact with persons with a mental disorder, estimated LMR) were selected, nearly the same variables used in Von dem Knesebeck et al. (2013), which also asked for respondents’ city of residence and was therefore a 7-factor model. The results for each term (“mental illness,” “mental disorder,” “mental health condition”) were combined to increase the overall numbers in the regression analysis.

Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to examine what effect terminology and informing respondents about LMR might have on the stigma scores. This resulted in a 3 (“mental illness,” mental disorder,” or “mental health condition”) × 2 (either “informed” about LMR at the beginning of the survey or “not informed” and instead asked to provide LMR estimates) fully-crossed factorial design.

Results

Estimates and stigma scores

Table 3 shows the results of multiple regression analyses performed on a 6-factor model. The estimates for LMR were significantly associated with respondents’ social distance scores. The 6-factor model in this study predicted 21 % of the variance in social distance scores. In contrast, the 7-factor model from Von dem Knesebeck et al. predicted only 3 % of the variance in social distance.

Table 3 Predictors of desired social distance from individuals with mental disorders among respondents not informed about prevalence rates (n = 151): results of multiple regression analyses

As seen in Table 2, social distance scores for the “not informed” groups were very similar to those for the “informed” groups. This suggests that simply informing respondents about high LMR did not lead to a significant reduction in their stigmatizing attitudes.

Effects of terminology

There was a main effect of terminology on social distance scores, F(2296) = 3.15, p = 0.044, and Tukey post hoc tests found that those assigned to the term “mental disorder” (n = 99) scored higher than those assigned to “mental health condition” (n = 104) (M diff  = 1.38, p = 0.038), but not “mental illness” (n = 99) (M diff  = 0.97, p = 0.20). There were no significant interaction effects found for any of the ANOVA comparisons and no changes in the regression model when effects of terminology were controlled.

Discussion

This study was intended to examine the relationship between estimates of the prevalence of mental disorders and social distance. The results suggest that higher estimates predict lower desire for social distance. The study was also intended to find the specific term (“mental illness,” “mental disorder,” or “mental health condition”) most likely to evoke these stigmatizing beliefs, and produced some evidence that the label “mental disorder” was more stigmatizing.

Terminology

The findings of greater stigma associated with the label “mental disorder” stand in contrast with the results from Szeto et al. (2013). It is possible that the term “mental disorder” is, in fact, associated with greater stigma, perhaps by emphasizing the otherness of those stigmatized. The negative findings from Szeto et al. could be related to their use of vignettes, which may have lessened the stigmatizing effects of the label. Participants in that study were also undergraduates receiving extra credit for a psychology course who could have been more familiar with the terms (Szeto et al. 2013). Future research is needed to examine the stigma associated with specific terms, and the way such terms are used and applied in the culture at large.

Estimates and association with stigma

Estimates for LMR were associated with social distance scores. The regression model shows that an increase of 1 % in a respondent’s prediction for the percentage of Americans who meet criteria for a mental disorder at some point in life (LMR) is associated with a 0.03 point decrease in the respondent’s score for desired social distance. Theoretically, increased LMR estimates from 33 to 100 % might be associated with a drop in social distance scores from the respondents’ mean of 12 to 10, though such assumptions risk extrapolation error. The standardized regression coefficients suggest that LMR estimates are 3/4 as predictive of desire for social distance as actually having a mental disorder, and almost half as predictive of desire for social distance as contact, which is reported to be the most effective anti-stigma intervention (Corrigan et al. 2012). Since increasing contact with individuals with mental disorders may be difficult, these results suggest that changing public beliefs in the LMR for any mental disorder may be a viable option for reducing stigma. If sizeable changes in public desire for social distance can be achieved through interventions as apparently simple as changing beliefs in the LMR for mental disorders, then they may become especially attractive to anti-stigma programs.

The positive results in this study and the negative findings in Von dem Knesebeck et al. may be related to their very different focus: all mental disorders in this study, and individual ones in Von dem Knesebeck et al. (2013). Conceptions about all mental disorders combined could be more related to beliefs held by some respondents about the universal nature of mental disorders. The different results could also reflect other methodological differences between the studies, such as telephone versus online format, use of vignettes, and different scales for social distance (Von dem Knesebeck et al. 2013). Differences in selection bias should also be considered. While 86 % of those who started this study completed and were included in the analysis, the response rate in Von dem Knesebeck et al. was only 51 %.

Implications for anti-stigma programs

These results suggest that anti-stigma interventions aimed at improving public awareness of the LMR for having any mental disorder hold promise. But they must do more than simply show prevalence estimates. An individual’s beliefs in high LMR may be accompanied by a reflective process that results in lower desire for social distance, and thinking that mental disorders are more common might make them seem more normal. Perhaps future programs may be able to reduce stigma by facilitating this reflective process. Such interventions could also be more successful in adolescents, who respond best with anti-stigma interventions that involve education rather than contact (Corrigan et al. 2012). Since MTurk is only available to adults, it cannot be used to study these younger populations.

Anti-stigma interventions that involve education or information are generally believed to be less effective than those involving contact, but the approaches are rarely studied in isolation. Meta-analyses of interventions for public stigma have found only 2 studies supporting the effectiveness of contact with no adjunct (Thornicroft et al. 2015), and only 1 randomized control trial was effective for the “mental illness” label (Griffiths et al. 2014). Education, its relationship with contacts, and possible mediating factors, such as disclosure, all deserve further study.

Limitations

The study had a number of limitations. The sample size was small. It was not drawn from the population at large and was conducted online via MTurk. MTurk samples do resemble the population, but they differ in some respects. The ability for MTurk workers to browse for tasks by their descriptions and keywords also creates the potential for bias. Yet few workers use keywords to find tasks, and it is the recency of a task that is posted, its length, and compensation rate, which was generous in this study, that generally determine if an MTurk worker will participate (Paolacci and Chandler 2014). Overall, the anonymous online format appears more likely to elicit the honest responses that are needed to measure these associations, without social desirability bias (Henderson et al. 2012).

Informing respondents about the high LMR of mental disorders did not reduce stigma, potentially limiting the significance of the associations. The strength of the associations in this study was also relatively modest. In the regression model, even the most dramatic changes in perceived LMR do not predict more than 2 points’ improvement in desired social distance. Yet even 1 point on the social distance scale used here is enough to make those probably unwilling to work closely on a job with an individual with a mental disorder, now probably willing. As with all research using social distance scales, however, behavioral responses must not be inferred from reported intentions. And social distance scales are only proxy measures for actual discrimination.

Lastly, stigma associated terms with terms such as “mental illness,” “mental disorder,” or “mental health condition,” will have no impact on the community if the terms are not applied to real behaviors and individuals. Only some disorders are identified as “mental illness” when described in vignettes: 88 % for schizophrenia, 69 % for major depressive disorder, 49 % for alcohol dependence, and 44 % for cocaine dependence (Link et al. 1999), but only 11.3 % for social anxiety disorder, 6.8 % for panic disorder, and 6.1 % for generalized anxiety disorder (Coles and Coleman 2010). Yet the stigma of “mental illness” will remain for as long as these labels are applied to such conditions.

Conclusions

The term “mental disorder” may be associated with relatively higher stigma than terms “mental illness” or “mental health condition.” Individuals who provided higher LMR estimates for having any mental disorder expressed less desire for social distance. But simply informing individuals about the high LMR of mental disorders does not appear to reduce the stigma associated with the conditions. Future research should identify variables mediating the associations between LMR estimates and stigma.

Even with increasing evidence from prospective studies to suggest the almost universal prevalence of mental disorders in the community, psychiatrists continue to debate the possible effects of accepting and disseminating such information to the public (Moffitt et al. 2010). Yet if stigma may in some way be reduced by accepting and promoting these epidemiological reports, why wait? Public beliefs in the LMR for any disorder must not be ignored for their potential to alter and reduce the public stigma of mental disorders.

References

  • American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edn. American Psychiatric Publishing, Arlington

    Google Scholar 

  • Bogardus ES (1959) Social distance. University of Southern California Press, Los Angeles

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourget B, Chenier R; Canadian Alliance on Mental Illness and Mental Health (2007) Mental health literacy in Canada: phase one draft report mental health literacy project. Ottawa: Canadian Alliance on Mental Illness and Mental Health. http://camimh.ca/wp-content/uploads/2012/04/Mental-Health-Literacy_-_Full-Final-Report_EN.pdf

  • Buhrmester M, Kwang T, Gosling SD (2011) Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: a new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspect Psychol Sci 6(1):3–5. doi:10.1177/1745691610393980

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clement S, Schauman O, Graham T, Maggioni F, Evans-Lacko S, Bezborodovs N, Morgan C, Rusch N, Brown JS, Thornicroft G (2015) What is the impact of mental health-related stigma on help-seeking? A systematic review of quantitative and qualitative studies. Psychol Med 45(1):11–27. doi:10.1017/S0033291714000129

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coles ME, Coleman SL (2010) Barriers to treatment seeking for anxiety disorders: initial data on the role of mental health literacy. Depress Anxiety 27(1):63–71. doi:10.1002/da.20620

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cook JA (2006) Employment barriers for persons with psychiatric disabilities: update of a report for the president’s commission. Psychiatr Serv 57(10):1391–1405. doi:10.1176/appi.ps.57.10.1391

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corrigan PW, Watson AC, Ottati WV (2003) From whence comes mental illness stigma? Int J Soc Psychiatry 49(2):142–157. doi:10.1177/0020764003049002007

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corrigan PW, Morris SB, Michaels PJ, Rafacz JD, Rusch N (2012) Challenging the public stigma of mental illness: a meta-analysis of outcome studies. Psychiatr Serv 63(10):963–973. doi:10.1176/appi.ps.201100529

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corrigan PW, Bink AB, Fokuo JK, Schmidt A (2015) The public stigma of mental illness means a difference between you and me. Psychiatr Res 226(1):186–191. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2014.12.047

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Department of Health (2003) Attitudes to mental illness. Taylor Nelson Sofres, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Economou M, Richardson C, Gramandani C, Stalikas A, Stefanis C (2009) Knowledge about schizophrenia and attitudes towards people with schizophrenia in Greece. Int J Soc Psychiatry 55(4):361–371. doi:10.1177/0020764008093957

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths KM, Carron-Arthur B, Parsons A, Reid R (2014) Effectiveness of programs for reducing the stigma associated with mental disorders. A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. World Psychiatry 13(2):161–175. doi:10.1002/wps.20129

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson C, Evans-Lacko S, Flach C, Thornicroft G (2012) Responses to mental health stigma questions: the importance of social desirability and data collection method. Can J Psychiatry 57(3):152–160. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.112.112979

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinshaw SP, Stier A (2008) Stigma as related to mental disorders. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 4:367–393. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.4.022007.141245

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones V, Corrigan PW (2014) Understanding stigma. In: The stigma of disease and disability: understanding causes and overcoming injustices. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, pp 9–34. doi:10.1037/14297-002

  • Kemali D, Maj M, Veltro F, Crepet P, Lobrace S (1989) Sondaggio sulle opinion degli italiani nei riguardi dei malati di mente e della situazione psichiatrica italiana [Survey on the Italian public’s opinions about mental disorders and psychiatric care]. Rivista Sperimentale di Freniatria 113(Suppl. 5):1301–1351

    Google Scholar 

  • Kessler RC, Wang PS (2008) The descriptive epidemiology of commonly occurring mental disorders in the United States. Annu Rev Public Health 29:115–129. doi:10.1146/annurev.publhealth.29.020907.090847

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kessler RC, Demler O, Frank RG, Olfson M, Pincus HA, Walters EE, Wang P, Wells KB, Zaslavsky AM (2005) Prevalence and treatment of mental disorders, 1990 to 2003. N Engl J Med 352(24):2515–2523. doi:10.1056/NEJMsa043266

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Link BG, Phelan JC (2001) Conceptualizing stigma. Annu Rev Sociol 27:363–385. doi:10.1146/annurev.soc.27.1.363

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Link BG, Phelan JC, Bresnahan M, Stueve A, Pescosolido BA (1999) Public conceptions of mental illness: labels, causes, dangerousness, and social distance. Am J Public Health 89(9):1328–1333. doi:10.2105/AJPH.89.9.1328

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin JK, Pescosolido BA, Tuch SA (2000) Of fear and loathing: the role of “disturbing behavior”, labels, and causal attributions in shaping public attitudes toward people with mental illness. J Health Soc Behav 41(2):208–223

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Taylor A, Kokaua J, Milne BJ, Polanczyk G, Poulton R (2010) How common are common mental disorders? Evidence that lifetime prevalence rates are doubled by prospective versus retrospective ascertainment. Psychol Med 40(6):899–909. doi:10.1017/S0033291709991036

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mojtabai R (2010) Mental illness and willingness to seek mental health care in the European Union. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 45(7):705–712. doi:10.1007/s00127-009-0109-2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mond JM, Hay PJ, Rodgers B, Owen C, Beaumont PJ (2004) Beliefs of women concerning the severity and prevalence of bulimia nervosa. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 39(4):299–304. doi:10.1007/s00127-004-0726-8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Comorbidity Survey Replication (2007). http://www.hcp.med.harvard.edu/ncs/ftpdir/table_ncsr_LTprevgenderxage.pdf. Accessed 10 January 2015

  • Nordt C, Rossler W, Lauber C (2006) Attitudes of mental health professionals toward people with schizophrenia and major depression. Schizophr Bull 32(4):709–714. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbj065

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nosek BA, Banaji MR, Jost JT (2009) The politics of intergroup attitudes. In: Jost JT, Kay AC, Thorisdottir H (eds) The social and psychological bases of ideology and system justification. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 480–506. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195320916.003.020

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Paolacci G, Chandler J (2014) Inside the Turk: understanding Mechanical Turk as a participant pool. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 23(3):184–188. doi:10.1177/0963721414531598

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paolacci G, Chandler J, Iperirotis PG (2010) Running experiments on Amazon Mechanical Turk. Judgm Decis Mak 5(5):411–419

    Google Scholar 

  • Pescosolido BA, Medina TR, Martin JK, Long JS (2013) The “backbone” of stigma: identifying the global core of prejudice associated with mental illness. Am J Public Health 103(5):853–860. doi:10.2105/AJPH.2012.301147

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips DL (1963) Rejection: a possible consequence of seeking help for mental disorders. Am Sociol Rev 28(6):963–972

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sartorius N (2004) The World Psychiatric Association global programme against stigma and discrimination because of schizophrenia. In: Crisp AH (ed) Every family in the land, revised edn. Royal Society of Medicine Press, London, pp 373–375

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheid TL (1999) Employment of individuals with mental disabilities: business response to the ADA’s challenge. Behav Sci Law 17(1):73–91. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0798(199901/03)17:1<73:AID-BSL326>3.0.CO;2-3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schulze B (2007) Stigma and mental health professionals: a review of the evidence on an intricate relationship. Int Rev Psychiatry 19(2):137–155. doi:10.1080/09540260701278929

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro DN, Chandler J, Mueller PA (2013) Using Mechanical Turk to study clinical populations. Clin Psychol Sci 1(2):213–220. doi:10.1177/2167702612469015

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith TW, Marsden PV, Hout M (2015) General Social Surveys, 1972-2014: cumulative codebook/Principal Investigator, Tom W. Smith; Co-Principal Investigators, Peter V. Marsden and Michael Hout. Chicago: National Opinion Research Center. (National Data Program for the Social Sciences Series, No. 23)

  • Szeto AC, Luong D, Dobson KS (2013) Does labeling mater? An examination of attitudes and perceptions of labels for mental disorders. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 48(4):659–671. doi:10.1007/s00127-012-0532-7

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Takayanagi Y, Spira AP, Roth KB, Gallo JJ, Eaton WW, Mojtabai R (2014) Accuracy of reports of lifetime mental and physical disorders: results from the Baltimore Epidemiological Catchment Area study. JAMA Psychiatry 71(3):273–280. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.3579

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thornicroft G, Mehta N, Clement S, Evans-Lacko S, Doherty M, Rose D, Koschorke M, Shidhaye R, O’Reilly C, Henderson C (2015) Evidence for effective interventions to reduce mental-health-related stigma and discrimination. Lancet. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(15)00298-6

    Google Scholar 

  • Von dem Knesebeck O, Mnich E, Kofahl C, Makowski AC, Lambert M, Karow A, Bock T, Harter M, Angermeyer MC (2013) Estimated prevalence of mental disorders and the desire for social distance–results from population surveys in two large German cities. Psychiatry Res 209(3):670–674. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2013.04.001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • World Health Organization (2001) The World Health report 2001—mental health: new understanding and hope. Geneva, World Health Organization. http://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/10665/42390/1/WHR_2001.pdf

  • World Psychiatric Association (2016) Stigma and Mental Illness. Geneva, World Psychiatric Association. http://www.wpanet.org/detail.php?section_id=11&content_id=555. Accessed 10 Jan 2016

  • Young Minds (2010) Stigma—a review of the evidence. Young Minds, London. https://www.youngminds.org.uk/assets/0000/1324/stigma-review.pdf

Download references

Competing interests

The author declares that he has no competing interests. The study protocol was determined to be exempt by the appropriate Institutional Review Board.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nicholas D. Lawson.

Additional file

40064_2016_2974_MOESM1_ESM.docx

Additional file 1: Table S1. Survey items. Table S2. Percentages of respondents endorsing a stigmatizing response on social distance items in the current study and in the Stigma in Global Context—Mental Health Study (SGC-MHS) and General Social Survey (GSS) depression and schizophrenia vignettes for the US and world public.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lawson, N.D. Public perception of the lifetime morbid risk of mental disorders in the United States and associations with public stigma. SpringerPlus 5, 1342 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40064-016-2974-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40064-016-2974-y

Keywords